The Annals of Applied Statistics

What we look at in paintings: A comparison between experienced and inexperienced art viewers

Anna-Kaisa Ylitalo, Aila Särkkä, and Peter Guttorp

Full-text: Access denied (no subscription detected)

We're sorry, but we are unable to provide you with the full text of this article because we are not able to identify you as a subscriber. If you have a personal subscription to this journal, then please login. If you are already logged in, then you may need to update your profile to register your subscription. Read more about accessing full-text

Abstract

How do people look at art? Are there any differences between how experienced and inexperienced art viewers look at a painting? We approach these questions by analyzing and modeling eye movement data from a cognitive art research experiment, where the eye movements of twenty test subjects, ten experienced and ten inexperienced art viewers, were recorded while they were looking at paintings.

Eye movements consist of stops of the gaze as well as jumps between the stops. Hence, the observed gaze stop locations can be thought of as a spatial point pattern, which can be modeled by a spatio-temporal point process. We introduce some statistical tools to analyze the spatio-temporal eye movement data, and compare the eye movements of experienced and inexperienced art viewers. In addition, we develop a stochastic model, which is rather simple but fits quite well to the eye movement data, to further investigate the differences between the two groups through functional summary statistics.

Article information

Source
Ann. Appl. Stat., Volume 10, Number 2 (2016), 549-574.

Dates
Received: October 2014
Revised: December 2015
First available in Project Euclid: 22 July 2016

Permanent link to this document
https://projecteuclid.org/euclid.aoas/1469199884

Digital Object Identifier
doi:10.1214/16-AOAS921

Mathematical Reviews number (MathSciNet)
MR3528351

Zentralblatt MATH identifier
06625660

Keywords
Coverage intensity point process shift function transition probability

Citation

Ylitalo, Anna-Kaisa; Särkkä, Aila; Guttorp, Peter. What we look at in paintings: A comparison between experienced and inexperienced art viewers. Ann. Appl. Stat. 10 (2016), no. 2, 549--574. doi:10.1214/16-AOAS921. https://projecteuclid.org/euclid.aoas/1469199884


Export citation

References

  • Anonymous (2000). Brochure of Art Centre Salmela. Mäntyharju, Finland.
  • Antes, J. R. (1974). The time course of picture viewing. J. Exp. Psychol. 103 62–70.
  • Baddeley, A., Rubak, E. and Turner, R. (2015). Spatial Point Patterns: Methodology and Applications with R. Chapman & Hall/CRC Press, London. Available at http://www.taylorandfrancis.com/books/details/9781482210200/.
  • Bailey, T. C. and Gatrell, A. C. (1995). Interactive Spatial Data Analysis. Longman, Harlow.
  • Barlow, H. B. (1952). Eye movements during fixation. J. Physiol. 116 290–306.
  • Barnard, G. A. (1963). Contribution to the discussion of professor Bartlett’s paper. J. Roy. Statist. Soc. Ser. A 25 294.
  • Barthelmé, S., Trukenbrod, H., Engbert, R. and Wichmann, F. (2013). Modelling fixation locations using spatial point processes. J. Vis. 13 1–34.
  • Berman, M. and Diggle, P. (1989). Estimating weighted integrals of the second-order intensity of a spatial point process. J. Roy. Statist. Soc. Ser. B 51 81–92.
  • Buswell, G. T. (1935). How People Look at Pictures—A Study of the Psychology of Perception in Art. The Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.
  • Diggle, P. J. (1985). A kernel method for smoothing point process data. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. C. 34 138–147.
  • Doksum, K. A. and Sievers, G. L. (1976). Plotting with confidence: Graphical comparisons of two populations. Biometrika 63 421–434.
  • Duchowski, A. T. (2002). A breadth-first survey of eye tracking applications. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 34 455–470.
  • Engbert, R., Trukenbrod, H. A., Barthelmé, S. and Wichmann, F. A. (2015). Spatial statistics and attentional dynamics in scene viewing. J. Vis. 15 1–17.
  • Findlay, J. M. (2009). Saccadic eye movement programming: Sensory and attentional factors. Psychol. Res. 73 127–135.
  • Gatrell, A. C., Bailey, T. C., Diggle, P. J. and Rowlingson, B. S. (1996). Spatial point pattern analysis and its application in geographical epidemiology. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, New Series 21 256–274.
  • Hazelton, M. L. (2007). Kernel estimation of risk surfaces without the need for edge correction. Stat. Med. 27 2269–2272.
  • Henderson, J. M. and Hollingworth, A. (1999). High-level scene perception. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 50 243–271.
  • Kelsall, J. E. and Diggle, P. J. (1995a). Kernel estimation of relative risk. Bernoulli 1 3–16.
  • Kelsall, J. E. and Diggle, P. J. (1995b). Non-parametric estimation of spatial variation in relative risk. Stat. Med. 14 2335–2342.
  • Kinsler, V. and Carpenter, R. H. S. (1995). Saccadic eye movements while reading music. Vis. Res. 35 1447–1458.
  • Komogartsev, O. V., Ryu, Y. S. and Koh, D. H. (2009). Quick models for saccade amplitude prediction. Journal of Eye Movement Research 1 1–13.
  • Kristjanson, A. F. and Antes, J. R. (1989). Eye movement analysis of artists and nonartists viewing paintings. Vis. Arts Res. 15 21–30.
  • Locher, P. J. (2006). The usefulness of eye movement recordings to subject an aesthetic episode with visual art to empirical scrutiny. Psychol. Sci. 48 106–114.
  • Mackworth, N. H. and Morandi, A. J. (1967). The gaze selects informative details within pictures. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2 547–552.
  • Manor, B. R. and Gordon, E. (2003). Defining the temporal threshold for ocular fixation in free-viewing visuocognitive tasks. J. Neurosci. Methods 128 85–93.
  • Mikkola, K., ed. (1997). Risto Suomi. Amos Anderson Art Museum, publications, new series, no 25.
  • Myllymäki, M., Mrkvicka, T., Grabarnik, P., Seijo, H. and Hahn, U. (2016). Global envelope tests for spatial processes. J. Roy. Statist. Soc. Ser. B. DOI:10.1111/rssb.12172.
  • Myllymäki, M., Grabarnik, P., Seijo, H. and Stoyan, D. (2015). Deviation test construction and power comparison for marked spatial point patterns. Spat. Stat. 11 19–34.
  • Nagasawa, S., Yim, S. and Hongo, H. (2005). Feasibility study on marketing research using eye movement: An investigation of image presentation using an “eye camera” and data processing. J. Adv. Comput. Intell. Intell. Informa. 9 440–452.
  • Noton, D. and Stark, L. (1971). Scanpaths in saccadic eye movements while viewing and recognizing patterns. Vis. Res. 11 929–942.
  • Penttinen, A. and Ylitalo, A.-K. (in press). Deducing self-interaction in eye movement data using sequential spatial point processes. Spatial Statistics. DOI:10.1016/j.spasta.2016.03.005.
  • The Yorck Project (2002). 10.000 Meisterwerke der Malerei, DVD-ROM, 2002. ISBN 3936122202. Distributed by DIRECTMEDIA Publishing GmbH.
  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 124 372–422.
  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., A Hum. Exp. Psychol. 62 1457–1506.
  • Ripley, B. D. (1988). Statistical Inference for Spatial Processes. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge.
  • Sundell, D. (1986). Eero Järnefelt (18631937), Retretti 25.5.–21.9.1986. Retretti.
  • Vogt, S. and Magnussen, S. (2007). Expertise in pictorial perception: Eye-movement patterns and visual memory in artists and laymen. Perception 36 91–100.
  • Wade, N. J. (2010). Pioneers of eye movement research. I-Perception 1 33–68.
  • Wakefield, J. C., Kelsall, J. E. and Morris, S. E. (2000). Clustering, cluster detection, and spatial variation in risk. In Spatial Epidemiology: Methods and Applications (P. Elliot, J. C. Wakefield, N. G. Best and D. J. Briggs, eds.) 128–152. Oxford Univ. Press, London.
  • Wilk, M. B. and Gnanadesikan, R. (1968). Probability plotting methods for the analysis of data. Biometrika 55 1–17.
  • Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye Movements and Vision. Plenum Press, New York.
  • Ylitalo, A.-K., Särkkä, A. and Guttorp, P. (2016a). Supplement to “What we look at in paintings: A comparison between experienced and inexperienced art viewers.” DOI:10.1214/16-AOAS921SUPPA.
  • Ylitalo, A.-K., Särkkä, A. and Guttorp, P. (2016b). Supplement to “What we look at in paintings: A comparison between experienced and inexperienced art viewers.” DOI:10.1214/16-AOAS921SUPPB.
  • Ylitalo, A.-K., Särkkä, A. and Guttorp, P. (2016c). Supplement to “What we look at in paintings: A comparison between experienced and inexperienced art viewers.” DOI:10.1214/16-AOAS921SUPPC.
  • Ylitalo, A.-K., Särkkä, A. and Guttorp, P. (2016d). Supplement to “What we look at in paintings: A comparison between experienced and inexperienced art viewers.” DOI:10.1214/16-AOAS921SUPPD.
  • Ylitalo, A.-K., Särkkä, A. and Guttorp, P. (2016e). Supplement to “What we look at in paintings: A comparison between experienced and inexperienced art viewers.” DOI:10.1214/16-AOAS921SUPPE.

Supplemental materials

  • Supplement A: Statistics for fixations inside the picture. Table includes information about the fixations, such as total number of fixations and mean fixation duration, for each subject.
  • Supplement B: Statistics for saccades inside the picture. Table includes information about the saccades, such as mean saccade duration and mean saccade length, for each subject.
  • Supplement C: The most visited areas during the six 30 second intervals for novices and non-novices. Figures show the top 5% and top 1% intensities in 30 second intervals (0–30 s), (30–60 s), (60–90 s), (90–120 s), (120–150 s), and (150–180 s) for novices and non-novices.
  • Supplement D: Results for the comparison of novices and non-novices for all six paintings. Results for the intensity surface and fixation duration distribution comparisons between novices and non-novices for all six paintings used in the experiment.
  • Supplement E: Results for the groupwise fixation duration distribution comparisons for three pairs of paintings. Results for the fixation duration distribution comparisons within novice and non-novice groups for the three pairs of paintings: Järnefelt–Monet, Kandinsky–Suomi, and Poussin–Tammi.